Theories of ‘nature vs. nurture’ have been debated since
before the turn of the nineteenth century, but professionals have yet to come
to a consensus regarding the matter. Perhaps
one of the best ways to study this phenomenon is by assessing and analyzing
adoptees. If either nature or nurture
were to have a distinguishably significant impact on character development, the
changes would be most evident in individuals who were raised by people other
than their biological parents, allowing for the separation of genetic and
environmental factors in the evaluation of their impact.
Various studies have been done to analyze the role of
adoption in child development, including studies regarding domestic and
international adoption. Research groups
such as van Londen et al.[1]
have demonstrated that the majority of internationally adopted infants are able
to form familial bonds of secure attachment following adoption. In addition,
adolescent and young adult adoptees have been studied regarding social
performance and externalization of behavior following cognitive maturation. One
of the longest studies analyzing the development of adopted children through
young adulthood is the Colorado Adoption Project (CAP)[2],
which has longitudinally studied behavioral development of adoptees since
1975. In this study, there are two
divisions of the analysis: Classic CAP, with subjects of ages 0-16, and Current
CAP, of subjects of ages 17 until now. The
studies of the CAP investigate
externalized effects on adopted subjects, such
as relationships, milestones, and education.
While studies of adoptees in their behavioral tendencies
have been done for a while, it was not until recently that studies were conducted
regarding the physiological changes and variations in reflexive responses to
stimuli. These types of studies arguably
fit into the ‘nature’ category of the nature vs. nurture debate. Schoenmaker et al.[3]
explores the influence of adoption experience on basic physiological reflexes
that are expected in young adults. The group looked at various aspects of
normal maternal/paternal instincts of adoptees, including attachment security,
perceived urgency, and respiratory sinus arrhythmia in the subjects. These variables were assessed with relation
to a cry paradigm, designed by the researchers to evoke certain reactions from
the participants. The most remarkable
impact identified was the ‘secure’ and ‘insecure’ attachment classification of
the individual. It was ultimately deemed that insecure attachment adoptees
responded with significantly lower urgency to the cry paradigm than securely
attached adoptees; furthermore, what can be described as a “higher tolerance”
for infant distress, decreasing the likelihood of a response.
In order to understand what the above conclusions are suggesting,
it is necessary to distinguish between secure and insecure attachment in
individuals. Secure attachment, as
outlined by Schoenmaker et al., secure attachment is rooted in a consistent
base of support beginning in the early developmental ages, instilling the
confidence that support will be there.
In contrast, an insecurely attached individual is someone who lacked
consistent and coherent support at a young age, and is therefore less motivated
by personal relationships and less likely to support others. Based on these definitions, along with the
adoptee status of participants, it can be argued that this is caused by
nurture.
The difficulty in determining whether the differences
between securely and insecurely attached adoptees stem from nature or nurture
lies in the juxtaposition of the explanations as to why the distinction
occurs. The potential difference in
physiological changes based on attachment representation addresses the
involvement of the parasympathetic nervous system in emotional and physiological
response to attachment stimuli (ie, the cry paradigm). The decreased emotionality displayed by the
insecurely attached individuals supports a distinction between secure and
insecure adoptees.
Studies of adopted individuals such as this one performed by
Shoenmaker et al. inform and suggest theories that could be useful to further
research. However, the clinical analysis
also adds another layer of complication to the nature vs. nurture debate. How can someone classify one way or the other
when both mechanisms have such an influence on the individual? This article
reinforces the battle between nature and nurture, which highlights the
difficulty in coming up with a decisive explanation. Many people interpret lack of concreteness to
be an indication of a marriage between the two theories. Regardless of the
medical, evolutionary, and scientific advances that have been made in the past
century, each study and article regarding human psychological development adds
complexity to the ongoing debate of ‘nature vs. nurture’ – a debate that does
not appear close to being resolved.
[1]
Londen, W van (11/2007). "Attachment, cognitive, and motor development in adopted children: short-term outcomes after international adoption". Journal of pediatric psychology (0146-8693), 32 (10), p. 1249.
[2]
Rhea, S.-A., Bricker, J. B., Wadsworth, S. J., & Corley, R. P. (2013). The Colorado Adoption Project. Twin Research and Human Genetics : The Official Journal of the International Society for Twin Studies, 16(1), 10.1017/thg.2012.109. doi:10.1017/thg.2012.109
[3]
Schoenmaker, C., Huffmeijer, R., van Ijzedoorn, MH., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., van den Dries, L., Linting, M., van der Voort, A., & Juffer, F. (2014). Attachment and physiological reactivity to infant crying in young adulthood: Dissociation between experiential and physiological arousal in insecure adoptees. Physiology & Behavior. pp. 549-556. Elsevier. *Main article on which this post was based
One point of conflict I have with the paper is that it's really hard to separate the effects of nature versus nurture. Without an identical twin to compare to, there's really know way of knowing for sure is there? Also, I wonder how the adopting parents' attitudes might change the child's physiology. That might drastically change adopting criteria.
ReplyDeleteI've always thought those who boil down changes in our physiology to "nature vs. nurture" aren't seeing the full picture. Both effect us, and those who say otherwise are simplifying it and ignoring the complexities of the world. I have certain traits because genetically they were passed down to me. Genes can determine intelligence, personality, and emotions to an extent. But these things are also shaped by our experience, how our caregivers treat us, and our education. I think boiling it down to an "either/or" doesn't respect the complexity of what makes us individuals.
ReplyDeleteI agree with Haley, I would think that the age of adoption would play a large role in the relationships that they form with their adoptive parents. I also agree with Max's comment in that how are researchers able to compare adpotees when there are so many differences between each of them. I would like to see a study that assesses identical twins that are raised in different environments (basically, recreate "The Parent Trap"). I think a lot of people would be more inclined to believe that nature v. nurture is a real thing if identical twins were compared because there is only one possible explanation for the differences observed.
ReplyDelete